The Science of When to Work

When do you do your research and writing?

With so many research students juggling jobs, childcare, and other responsibilities, we often can’t put in a standard 9-5 day. In a recent article about the AUT security team on AUTi (our intranet site), Security Manager Rehan du Toit reported that 30 PhD students and researchers worked through the night last New Year’s Eve!

Unconventional hours can be a necessity. Fortunately, one of the small luxuries of postgrad work is that it can often be done whenever you have the time or inspiration. Many facilities are available 24/7, and research materials such as books and articles are commonly available online. If the best time for you to work is at 2am, then usually, you can.

However, the times you have available may not necessarily be the times when your brain is at its best. Although everyone is different, we all follow a roughly 24-hour cycle called a ‘circadian rhythm’. These rhythms are thought to be partly externally influenced, as our bodies react to shifts in light levels and temperature. But they are also innate.

In a Harvard study, participants were put onto a schedule which divided their week into 6 days of 28 hours, rather than 7 days of 24 hours. That meant that the participants were awake and asleep at different times on different days. Despite their altered schedules, the participants’ bodies (as measured by their body temperature and hormone levels) continued to operate on a roughly 24-hour cycle (Czeisler et al, 1999). That shows that circadian rhythms are not solely a response to external cues; there is something in us that simply needs to function on a 24-hour cycle.

Cognitive scientists have linked circadian rhythms to brain function. One review paper looked at a wide range of studies on cognitive function at different times of day. The review focused on the three main aspects of cognitive processing: attention, memory, and executive function (which includes activities such as planning and categorising information). Here’s what they found:

  • Attention: our attentiveness stays fairly stable for up to 16 hours during the day, with an observable peak around midday, but drops off significantly at nighttime (Schmidt et al, 2007, p.766-767)
  • Memory: short-term memory appears to peak earlier in the day; overall, memory seems to decline at night, even when controlling for the amount of time spent awake (Schmidt et al, 2007, p.769-770)
  • Executive function: our ability to control our responses is best in the early afternoon and evening; but overall, executive functions are disproportionately affected by sleep deprivation (Schmidt et al, 2007, p.771)

Here’s the good news: the review found agreement among several studies which reported that cognitive tasks involving automatic processes are not significantly impacted by circadian rhythms (Schmidt et al, 2007, p.772). So if you need to perform a basic or repetitive task like filing papers or organising notes, you can probably get away with doing it when you’re not at your best.

However, there is huge individual variation. You might be a morning person or a night owl, or neutral, depending on genetic and social factors. So your optimal time of day might not fit the average.

One really interesting study found that an individual’s non-optimal time of day might actually be a better time for certain types of problem-solving. Wieth & Zacks (2011) had participants solve problems at different times of day, depending on the participant’s individual self-reported ‘optimal’ and ‘non-optimal’ times. As you might expect, participants were able to solve analytic problems (those requiring calculation or logic) more effectively during their optimal times. But for insight problems (those requiring creative thinking), the non-optimal times were actually better. So if you need to think outside the box, you might be wise to try thinking when you otherwise wouldn’t want to!

 

TL/DR: the science on cognitive function at different times of day tends to show that people function better on average during daytime; but individual variation means that your own preference for particular times of day is very important too. Even during your ‘off’ times, you can still effectively perform some tasks (such as repetitive or basic tasks, or creative problem-solving).

 

Sources

Czeisler, C. A., Duffy, J. F., Shanahan, T. L., Brown, E. N., Mitchell, J. F., Rimmer, D. W., Ronda, J. M., Silva, E. J., Allan, J. S., Emens, J. S., Dijk, D. J., & Kronauer, R. E. (1999). Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science, 284(5423), 2177-2181. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5423.2177

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive neuropsychology, 24(7), 755-789. doi: 10.1080/02643290701754158

Wieth, M. B., & Zacks, R. T. (2011). Time of day effects on problem solving: When the non-optimal is optimal. Thinking & Reasoning, 17(4), 387-401. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2011.625663

About Anaise Irvine

Dr Anaise Irvine is the Editor of Thesislink and leads the Researcher Education and Development team at Auckland University of Technology. Her PhD research analysed how contemporary films and novels represent genetic engineering as a social justice issue. These days she works with researchers at all levels to improve their research skills, and the most obscure of her own research skills is being able to turn novels into phylogenetic trees!

2 thoughts on “The Science of When to Work

  1. Interesting! A couple of weeks ago I could find the link between some research components that I was struggling to link together. That happened when I was EXTREMELY exhausted, I I had a sleepless night the night before and was running around like crazy trying to finish some important tasks that cannot be delayed!
    So yeah, sometimes when your brain is forced to do a lot in spite of exhaustion it gets mixed us and does a lot more useful stuff 🙂 Thanks Anaise for the interesting topics!

    1. You’re living, breathing evidence for the idea that non-optimal time can be good for creative problem-solving. I’m glad that your exhaustion had a positive side-effect – though hopefully you’ve had a rest since then!

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

 characters available